
fa0bd59faa64d61e411613bf79005c0a.ppt
- Количество слайдов: 62
EDPSY 520: Psychology of Reading Virtual Overview and Introduction
Welcome n This introduction is intended to provide a brief history of the field and familiarize you with major issues in the psychology of reading n You will also have the opportunity to participate in some classic experiments in the study of reading processes n Click your way through this presentation via the mouse or spacebar; during some of the experiments the computer may take control of the screen pacing
Brief History n Psychological research on reading was done in the early 1900 s by E. B. Huey and others n According to Huey (1908): “. . . to completely analyze what we do when we read would almost be the acme of a psychologist’s achievements, for it would be to describe very many of the most intricate workings of the human mind. . ”
n With the rise of Behaviorism in the early part of the last century, especially within American psychology, the focus shifted to studying behaviors that were more observable than reading (more simple learning and more simple learners) n As cognitive psychology became increasingly dominant during the latter half of the century, reading (and human thinking in general) again became a phenomenon to study
How might we think about reading? n Perhaps because of lasting effects of Behaviorism, many early models of reading still had a very “stimulus-driven, ” or “textdriven” feel n In these early models, the assumption was that information in the text is taken in by the reader n Information was seen to flow from the “bottom up”
An Early Bottom-Up Model of Reading, La. Berge & Samuels (1972) n Letters are perceived as A visual patterns, and with learning, come to be recognized as letters n Letters are mentally P+A+T PAT combined and recognized as words n Words are successively PAT THE DOG ON recognized as phrases, THE HEAD. sentences, etc.
Not the whole story n To test such a text-driven model, you can now be a subject in an experiment that Edmund Burke Huey performed 100 years ago (with slightly different technology) n When you click the mouse or spacebar, you will see an asterisk ( * ) appear on the screen to orient you. A vertical list of items (letters or words) will then appear under the asterisk n See how many of the items you can read in the time you are given (if possible, read out loud, in a rapid but comfortable pace)
*
* y w u s q o m k i g e c
Did you finish the whole list? Did you have time left over? Click when you are ready to start the next list.
*
* pool rugs mark send list more pick stab neck your dice font
Did you finish the whole list? Did you have time left over? Click when you are ready to start the next list.
*
* analysis habitual occupied inherent probable summoned devotion remarked overcome resolute elements conclude
Did you finish the whole list? n If you are like most adult readers, you had time to read all the items on all the lists n Click to advance each screen again
What does this demonstrate? n We do not read letter by letter, building up words from individual letters n The lists contained successively more letters n n n List 1: 1 letter per line List 2: 4 letters per line List 3: 8 letters per line n However, the lists were not displayed for proportionately longer times n All lists were displayed for 8 seconds
You just replicated classic findings n You read the short words (4 letter words in List 2) just as quickly as you read single letters (List 1); both were displayed for only 8 seconds n Even though List 3 contained eight times more letters than List 1, you did not need eight times longer to read it n In studies with more precise timing, people even read short words more quickly than single letters, a phenomenon that has come to be called the “word superiority effect”
Conclusions n Your reading times just demonstrated that not all information flows from the text to the reader; not all information flows from the bottom up.
Alternative models of reading n Theoretical models emphasizing the expectations and prediction abilities of readers have come to be known as “top-down models”
Top-down models n Perhaps the purest of top-down models was offered by Ken Goodman, based on his analysis of readers’ errors as they read aloud, which he called “miscues” n Goodman noted that when they misread a word, good readers are more likely than poor readers to substitute a word that makes sense in the sentence context
Examples of reading errors, or “miscues” n When reading the sentence, “I walked up the sidewalk, across the porch, and knocked on the door of the house” n Good readers may misread the word house as home (but note how the sentence still makes sense) n Poor readers are more likely to misread the word house as horse, or how, or even as a nonsense word like “hoose” (rhyming with choose)
Other evidence of top-down processing n Semantic priming effects n We read the word doctor more quickly following the word nurse, chair more quickly following the word table, compared to when these words follow words with which they share no meaningful relationship
Other evidence of top-down processing n Prior knowledge effects n When interpreting sentences containing ambiguous words, such as “ When Jerry, Mike, and Pat arrived, Karen was sitting in her living room writing some notes” College students majoring in music interpreted the word notes as musical notes n Other students interpreted the words notes as brief letters or reminders to oneself n
Not the whole story either n Of course, top-down processing can’t be the whole story either because we do read, not simply daydream in front of books n The context that drives expectations comes from somewhere
Interactive Models of Reading n Most reading researchers now adopt a theoretical model that includes interaction from both topdown and bottom-up processes
Implications of an Interactive Model n Interactive models are not a simple compromise, not a little of both n Interactive reading processes require complex coordination of multiple sources of information The text on the page n Context from previous text n Prior knowledge n Reading goals n
Implications of an Interactive Model n Where do we store all these sources of information? n How do we coordinate multiple information sources, accessing them seemingly simultaneously?
Unlimited Long-term Memory (LTM) n Every kind of knowledge we use as part of reading must be stored in our long-term memory (LTM) Letters n Word meanings n Memory for general story structures n Memory for a specific text n Prior knowledge of the topic n
Limited Short-term Memory (STM) and Attention n The amount of information we can hold in immediate STM at any given time is limited n The storage capacity of STM has been estimated between 5 and 9 (7+2), but there are strategies to increase STM n Still, STM limits generally exist; to convince yourself, try this multiplication problem in your head: 1683945 x 6939
Implications of an Interactive Model n During reading, attention can’t be everywhere at once, even in an interactive system n If attention is required to identify individual letters, then less attention is available to devote to recognizing words n If attention is required to recognize words, then less attention is available to devote to building up story context
How do Interactions Work? n The key to getting around limits on attention and STM is to have some processes (such as letter and word recognition) go on automatically n Automaticity is the underlying cause of the word superiority effect you demonstrated for yourself some while back
How do Automatic Processes Work? n With the next few slides you can take part in another classic reading experiment n Click to begin
Name the color of each letter cluster Read the following clusters from left to right, as though you were reading connected sentences. Remember to name the color of the ink. Click to begin
Name the color of each letter cluster
Name the color of each letter cluster XXXXX XXX XXXX XXX XXX XXXXX XXX
Did you finish the whole set? Did you have time left over? Click when you are ready to start the next set.
Name the color of each letter cluster Read the following clusters from left to right, as though you were reading connected sentences. Remember to name the color of the ink. Click to begin
Name the color of each letter cluster
Name the color of each letter cluster GREEN BLUE RED GREEN RED BLUE GREEN RED BLUE RED GREEN BLUE RED
Did you finish the whole set? n If you are like most adult readers, you named the color of ink far more easily in the first set than the second n In the second list, you may have experienced strong interference because you wanted to say aloud the word that you saw, rather than name the color of the ink in which the words were printed
What does this demonstrate? n Your word reading processes are automatic n The good news: Automatic processes run without conscious attention or effort n The bad news: Automatic processes are also hard to “turn off” n You just replicated another classic study in reading research, illustrating the “Stroop effect” (named for the experimenter who first reported it)
Implications for an Interactive Model n If some of the multiple processes we are trying to coordinate are automatic, they can operate without tying up much attention or STM capacity n Thus, automatic lower level (bottom up) process such as letter and word identification can free up limited STM capacity for higher level (top down) processes, such as comprehension, interpretation, etc.
Reading Problems n What are the most common sources of reading problems? n Are less skilled readers poorer at bottom up processes such as word identification? n Or, are less skilled readers necessarily poorer at using context (as Goodman suggested)?
Another classic experiment in reading n Perfetti & Roth examined the use of context by skilled and less skilled readers in Grade 4 n Children listened to short stories and were timed as they read aloud just the final word n Thus, the beginning of each story provided context for the final word, which was the only word that children had to read n However, the predictive quality of the context provided by the story varied
Perfetti and Roth’s study n Some contexts were highly predictable n Children heard: The garbage men had loaded as much as they could onto the truck. They would have to drop off a load at the garbage _____. n Children read: dump
Perfetti and Roth’s study n Some contexts were less predictable n Children heard: Albert didn’t have the money he needed to buy the part to fix his car. Luckily he found the part he wanted at the _____. n Children read: dump
Perfetti and Roth’s study n Some contexts were anomalous (misleading) n Children heard: Phil couldn’t decide whether to go to the movies or to the party. Both sounded like lots of fun, but he finally decided to go to the _____. n Children read: dump
How do skilled readers use context? n Perfetti & Roth measured in milliseconds how long children in the two groups needed to read aloud the final word of each story n If skilled readers use context to help identify words, what do you predict will happen to their reading times as they go from highly predictable to less predictable contexts? n What about as they go to anomalous contexts? Click to see their actual results
Time to Read Final Word Aloud Skilled 4 th grade readers High Less predictable Anomalous Predictability of Context
What do these results tell us? n Skilled readers use context n Skilled readers read words fastest when the context was highly predictive n When contexts were less predictive or anomalous, skilled readers took almost 100 milliseconds longer to read the same word
How do less-skilled readers use context? n If less skilled readers do not use context to help identify words, what do you predict will happen to their reading times as they go from highly predictable to less predictable contexts? n What about as they go to anomalous contexts? n Click to see their actual results
Time to Read Word Aloud Skilled 4 th grade readers High Less predictable Anomalous Predictability of Context
Time to Read Word Aloud Less skilled 4 th grade readers Skilled 4 th grade readers High Less predictable Anomalous Predictability of Context
What do these results tell us? n Less skilled readers use context too! n Less skilled readers read words fastest when the context was highly predictive n When contexts were less predictive they took almost 150 milliseconds longer to read the same word n When contexts were anomalous, they took another 150 milliseconds longer to read the same word
Who uses context more? n Less skilled readers were even more sensitive to context than skilled readers n The slope of the less skilled readers’ reading times was steeper as context became less predictive n Less skilled readers were especially hesitant to read a word when it violated the context
Who uses context more? n This does not mean that skilled readers don’t use context n In all but the most predictable contexts, skilled readers identify words so fast that they don’t need to use context
Who uses context more? n How do these results fit with Goodman’s observations about poor readers not using context as they read? n In Perfetti and Roth’s study, the less skilled readers did not have to read the context; they got it “for free” by listening n The problem for less skilled readers is keeping context in mind when they have to read it for themselves
Implications of an Interactive Model n During reading, attention can’t be everywhere at once, even in an interactive system n If less skilled readers’ attention is required to identify individual words, then less attention is available to devote to building up story context n Less skilled readers are in a double bind; they rely on context more than skilled readers, but can’t maintain it while reading
The story is more complicated n Reading is a complex interaction between bottom up and top down processes n The experiments you have reviewed here do not tell the complete story n Not all readers have difficulties for exactly the same reasons n Readers differ in terms of the processing skill and the knowledge they bring to reading
More to come n Much of our work for the rest of the term will examine: On what knowledge does reading build n How do knowledge and processes interact n How do skilled and less skilled readers differ n
See you at our first meeting